You are here
قراءة كتاب A Field Study of the Kansas Ant-Eating Frog, Gastrophryne olivacea
تنويه: تعرض هنا نبذة من اول ١٠ صفحات فقط من الكتاب الالكتروني، لقراءة الكتاب كاملا اضغط على الزر “اشتر الآن"

A Field Study of the Kansas Ant-Eating Frog, Gastrophryne olivacea
align="left">July 20-21
.94 3 July 24 .38 2 July 28 .29 none August 1-2 3.22 31 August 6-7-8 2.43 none August 12 .28 none August 16 .29 none August 19-22 .70 none August 27-28 1.05 none September 9 .50 none September 29-30 .38 none October 4 .74 none October 12-14 3.51 noneFrom the positions of the traps and drift fences, it was obvious that all of the frogs that were caught were travelling toward the pond. Capture of an equal number moving away from the pond a few days afterward might have been expected but none at all was caught while making a return trip. Therefore it seems that the frogs returned by a different route to their home ranges after breeding. Of necessity they make the return trip under conditions drier than those that prevail on the pondward trip, which is usually made in a downpour. Probably the return travel is slower, more leisurely, and with more tendency to keep to sheltered situations.
The call is a bleat, resembling that of a sheep, but higher, of lesser volume, and is not unlike the loud rattling buzz of an angry bee. The call is usually of three to four seconds duration, with an interval
several times as long. Calling males were floating, almost upright, in the water within a few yards of shore, where there was dense vegetation. The throat pouch when fully expanded is several times as large as the entire head. When a person approached to within a few yards of frogs they usually stopped calling, submerged, and swam to a place of concealment.
Having heard the call of typical G. carolinensis in Louisiana, I have the impression that it is a little shorter, more sheeplike, and less insectlike than that of G. olivacea. The call of Gastrophryne is of such peculiar quality that it is difficult to describe. Different observers have described it in different terms. Stebbins (1951: 391) has described the call in greatest detail, and also has quoted from the descriptions of it previously published. These descriptions include the following: "high, shrill buzz"; "buzz, harsh and metallic"; "like an electric buzzer"; "like bees at close range but more like sheep at a distance"; "bleating baa"; "shrill, long-drawn quaw quaw"; "whistled whēē followed by a bleat."
Stebbins observed breeding choruses (mazatlanensis) at Peña Blanca Springs, Arizona, and stated that sometimes three or four called more or less together, but that they seldom started simultaneously. Occasionally many voices would be heard in unison followed by an interval of silence, but this performance was erratic. At the pond on the Reservation I noted this same tendency many times. After a lull the chorus would begin with a few sporadic croaks, then four or five or even more frogs would be calling simultaneously from an area of a few square yards. Anderson (op. cit.: 34) found that in small groups of calling G. carolinensis there was a distinct tendency to maintain a definite pattern in the sequence of the calls. One "dominant" individual would initiate a series of calls, and others each in turn would take up the chorus.
Pairing takes place soon after the breeding aggregations are formed. On the night of June 4, 1954, a clasping pair was captured and kept in the laboratory in a large jar of water. This pair did not separate, and spawning occurred between noon and 1:30 P. M. on June 5. When the newly laid eggs were discovered at 1:30 P. M. most of them were in a surface film. Some were attached to submerged leaves and a few rested on the bottom. The pair was still joined, but the male was actually clasping only part of the time, and as the frogs moved about in the water, it became evident that they were adhering to each other by the areas of skin contact, which were glued together by their dermal secretion. They were unable to separate immediately, even when they struggled to do so. They
were observed for approximately 15 minutes before separation occurred, and during this time they were moving about actively. As they separated, the area of adhesion was discernible on the back of the female. It was U-shaped, following the ridges of the ilia and the sacrum.
On August 2, 1954, after a rain of 3.22 inches, the previously mentioned funnel trap in the ditch had caught 31 ant-eating frogs. Water had collected to a depth of several inches in the depression where the trap was situated. A dozen of the trapped frogs were clasping pairs. These frogs struggled vigorously as they were removed from the traps, handled and marked. As a result most of the clasping males were separated from the females. In handling those of each pair I noticed that they were glued together by dermal secretions, as were those of the pair observed on June 5. The areas of adhesion were of similar shape and location in the different pairs, and included the U-shaped ridge of the female's back and the male's belly, and the inner surfaces of the male's forelegs with the corresponding surfaces of the female's sides where the male clasped.
This adhesion of the members of a pair during mating may be a normal occurrence. The copious secretion of the dermal glands is of especially glutinous quality in Gastrophryne. The adhesion of members of a pair may have survival value. These small frogs are especially shy, and in the breeding ponds they respond to any disturbance with vigorous attempts to escape and hide. Under such circumstances the adhesion may prevent separation. Also, it may serve to prevent displacement of a clasping male by a rival. Anderson (op. cit.) who observed many details of the mating behavior of G. carolinensis, both in the laboratory and under natural conditions, mentioned no such adhesion between members of a pair.
Anderson (op. cit.: 31) discussed the possibility that reproductive isolation might arise in sympatric populations, such as those of G. carolinensis in southern Louisiana, through inherent differences in time of spawning. However, in G. olivacea at least, such isolation would be prevented by individual males returning to breed at different times in the same season. Furthermore, individual differences in choice of breeding time probably result from environmental factors rather than genetic factors in most instances. In G. olivacea in Kansas, time of breeding is controlled by the distribution of heavy rainfall creating favorable conditions. Onset of the breeding season may be hastened or delayed, or an entire year may be missed because of